Motivation

From My Strategy
Jump to navigation Jump to search

Back to Action fields - Organisational futuring


Welcome to the Motivation page

Description

What is Motivation?

The Latin root of the word “motivation” means “to move”; hence, in this basic sense the study of motivation is the study of action. Modern theories of motivation focus more specifically on the relation of beliefs, values, and goals with action.

.

Thinking about the concept

A visual thesaurus search is always an excellent starting point to discuss a concept definition:

MOTIVATION
Thesaurus - ENG
Thesaurus - Synonyms, Antonyms, and Related Words (freethesaurus.com)

.


.

Relations of Motivation within Organisational futuring (and Corporate futuring)

EGM - Of - Motivation - 3D - ENG.jpg

There exists a close vertical relationship within Organisational futuring:

On the Inner focus level, an affiliation exists with:

Corporate futuring and Organisational futuring intertwine, on the Inner focus level, through:

In concrete terms, this means:

  • The trio - 'Motivation', 'Talent development' and 'Human capital' - is inextricably linked.
  • Your 'Motivation' contributes substantially to 'Collaboration', 'Managing' and 'Governance'.
  • Furthermore, 'Motication' is part of the Inner focus level where 'Organisational-' and 'Corporate futuring' meet.

.


.

Questions we can ask ourselves when contemplating Motivation

  • Supports our 'Motivation' our 'Collaboration', our 'Managing' and our 'Governance'?
  • Is our 'Motivation' robust enough to generate a vialble 'Talent development'?
  • Is our 'Motivation' keen enough to support our 'Long term thinking' and 'Appreciation'?
  • Are our 'Motivation'-al actions a collider or a mediator between our 'Collaboration' and 'Governance'?

.


.

Theories about motivation

Theories focused on expectancy

Self-Efficacy Theory

Bandura proposed a social cognitive model of motivation focused on the role of perceptions of efficacy and human agency. Bandura defined self-efficacy as individuals' confidence in their ability to organise and execute a given course of action to solve a problem or accomplish a task. He characterised it as a multidimensional construct that varies in strength, generality, and level.

As in expectancy-value theory and attribution theory, Bandura's self-efficacy theory focuses on expectancies for success. However, Bandura distinguished between two kinds of expectancy beliefs: outcome expectations—beliefs that certain behaviours will lead to specific outcomes —and efficacy expectations—beliefs about whether one can effectively perform the behaviours necessary to produce the result. Indeed, Bandura proposed that individuals' efficacy expectations are the primary determinant of goal setting, activity choice, willingness to expend effort and persistence.

.

Control Theories

Locus of control theory is another type of expectancy-based theory. One should expect to succeed to the extent that one feels in control of one's successes and failures. Evidence supports this prediction. Recent locus of control theorists have elaborated broader conceptual models of control. They hypothesised that the extent to which needs are fulfilled is influenced by following characteristics of their contexts: the amount of structure, the degree of autonomy provided, and the level of involvement. Finally, they proposed that how these needs are fulfilled determines engagement in different activities. When the needs are fulfilled, we will be fully engaged. Agency beliefs are the expectations that one has access to the means needed to produce various outcomes. Control beliefs are the expectations individuals have that they can produce desired events.

.

Theories focused on the reason for engagement

Intrinsic Motivation Theories

Self determination theory

Given the growing evidence that extrinsic incentives and pressures can undermine motivation to perform even inherently interesting activities, Deci & Ryan proposed self-determination theory in which they integrated two perspectives on human motivation: humans are motivated to maintain an optimal level of stimulation, and humans have basic needs for competence and personal causation or self-determination. They argued that people seek out optimal stimulation and challenging activities and find these activities intrinsically motivating because they have a basic need for competence.

In addition, they argued that intrinsic motivation is maintained only when actors feel competent and self-determined. Consider, for example, a person who consciously and without any external pressure selects a specific job because it will help him earn money. This person is guided by his basic needs for competence and self-determination, but his choice of the job is based on reasons extrinsic to the job itself. Finally, Deci&Ryan postulated that a basic need for interpersonal relatedness explains why people turn external goals into internal goals through internalisation.

.

Flow theory

Csikszentmihalyi defined intrinsically motivated behaviour as the immediate subjective experience that occurs when people are engaged in an activity and feel in control of one's actions and the environment.

Flow is only possible when a person feels that the opportunities for action in a given situation match their ability to master the challenges. The challenge of an activity may be concrete or physical, like the peak of a mountain to be scaled or abstract and symbolic, like a set of musical notes to be performed, a story to be written, or a puzzle to be solved. Deci & Ryan conceptualise intrinsic motivation regarding innate, basic needs, whereas Csikszentmihalyi stresses subjective experience.

We suggest, however, that this difference reflects two sides of the same coin. First, Schneider has argued that one must distinguish between immediate and ultimate reasons for behaviour. Intrinsically motivated behaviour can be conducive to ultimate goals even though the actor is only motivated by direct incentives. This distinction between immediate and ultimate causes of behaviour makes it possible to reconcile the positions of Deci & Ryan and Csikszentmihalyi. Deci & Ryan focus on ultimate reasons for behaviour, whereas Csikszentmihalyi focuses on immediate causes. Second, Csikszentmihalyi & Massimini have suggested that the flow experience is a reward that ensures that individuals seek to increase their competence.

.

Individual difference theories of intrinsic motivation

Until recently, intrinsic motivation researchers have focused primarily on conditions, components, and consequences of inherent motivation without distinguishing between inherent motivation as a state versus intrinsic motivation as a trait-like characteristic.

These researchers define this enduring intrinsic motivational orientation as the preference for demanding or challenging tasks, learning driven by curiosity or interest, and striving for competence and mastery. Of course, both preferences for challenging tasks and striving for competence can be linked to either extrinsic or general need-achievement motivation. Nonetheless, empirical findings suggest that the three components are highly correlated.

.

Interest Theories

We differentiate between individual and situational interest. Value-related valences refer to the attribution of personal significance or importance to an object or activity. In addition, both feeling-related and value-related valences are directly related to the object or activity rather than the relation of this object or action to other objects or events. For example, if people associate mathematics with high personal significance because mathematics can help them get prestigious jobs, we would not speak of interest. Although feeling-related and value-related valences are highly correlated, it is helpful to differentiate between them because some individual interests are likely based primarily on feelings. In contrast, other interests are more likely to be based on personal significance. Much research on individual interest has focused on its relation to the quality of learning.

.

Goal Theories

Bandura and Schunk have shown that specific, proximal, and somewhat challenging goals promote self-efficacy and improved performance. Other researchers have defined and investigated broader goal orientations. For example, Nicholls and his colleagues described two significant motivationally relevant goal patterns or orientations: ego-involved goals and task-involved goals. Individuals with ego-involved goals seek to maximise favourable evaluations of their competence and minimise negative competence evaluations. Task-involved people choose challenging tasks and are more concerned with their progress than with outperforming others.

A significant advance in this area is distinguishing between performance-approach and performance-avoid goals. This distinction arose partly because of some inconsistent evidence about the effects of performance goals on various outcomes. As the name implies, performance-approach goals mean engagement in achievement tasks for performance reasons, whereas performance-avoid goals concern disengagement not to appear stupid. Generally, performance-approach goals seem to have more positive consequences on motivation and achievement than performance-avoid goals.

Personal goals include affective goals, cognitive goals, and subjective organisation goals. The person-environment goals include self-assertive goals such as self-determination and individuality, integrative social relationship goals such as belonging and social responsibility, and task goals such as mastery, material gain, and safety.

.

Theories integrating expectancy and value construct

Attribution Theory

Attribution theorists emphasise that individuals' interpretations of their achievement outcomes, rather than motivational dispositions or actual outcomes, determine subsequent achievement strivings. However, Weiner argued that the individual's causal attributions for achievement outcomes determine subsequent achievement strivings and, thus, are critical motivational beliefs.

Weiner and his colleagues identified ability, effort, task difficulty, and luck as the most crucial achievement attributions. The stability dimension captures whether causes change over time or not. For instance, ability was classified as a stable, internal cause, and effort was classified as unstable and internal. Finally, the locus of control dimension is linked most strongly to affective reactions.

.

Modern Expectancy-Value Theory

In this model, both negative and positive task characteristics are assumed to influence choices. All choices are considered to have costs associated with them precisely because one choice often eliminates other options. Consequently, the relative value and probability of success of various options are key determinants of choice. Expectancies and values are assumed to influence performance, persistence, and task choice directly.

However, empirical work has shown that children and adolescents do not distinguish between these two different levels of beliefs. Even though these constructs can be theoretically distinguished from each other, they are highly related and empirically indistinguishable in real-world achievement situations.

Eccles et al. outlined four components of task-value: attainment value, intrinsic value, utility value, and cost. Like Battle, they defined attainment value as the personal importance of doing well on the task. Drawing on self-schema and identity theories, they also linked attainment value to the relevance of engaging in a task for confirming or disconfirming salient aspects of one's self-schema.

Cost is conceptualized in terms of the negative aspects of engaging in the task, such as performance anxiety and fear of failure and success, as well as the amount of effort needed to succeed and the lost opportunities resulting from making one choice rather than another.

In his general expectancy-value model, Heckhausen attempted to integrate several different approaches to motivation. The resulting model distinguished four different types of expectancies: situation-outcome, action-outcome, action-by-situation-outcome, and outcome-consequence. It is important to note that in Heckhausen's model, outcomes are the immediate results of one's actions. These immediate results are or are not followed by various consequences. They do not have any incentive value on their own. Incentive value is only attributed to the effects of one's actions. Therefore, the motivation to act depends mainly on the value attached to the consequences of one's behaviour.

Like attribution theory, goal theory, and self-efficacy theory, modern expectancy-value theory can be criticized for emphasizing the rational cognitive processes leading to motivation and behaviour.

.

Self-Worth Theory

In his self-worth theory, Covington defined the motive for self-worth as the tendency to establish and maintain a positive self-image or sense of self-worth. Because we are evaluated so frequently, Covington argued that a critical way to maintain a sense of self-worth is to protect one's sense of competence. However, evaluation, competition, and social comparison make it difficult for many to maintain the belief that they are competent.

Covington discussed the strategies many develop to avoid appearing to lack ability. These include procrastination, making excuses, avoiding challenging tasks, and, perhaps most important, not trying. Covington suggested that reducing the frequency and salience of competitive, social comparative, and evaluative practices and focusing instead on effort, mastery, and improvement would allow more to maintain their self-worth without resorting to these failure-avoiding strategies.

.

Theories integrating motivation and cognition

Social Cognitive Theories of Self-Regulation and Motivation

In sum, the social cognitive view of self-regulation emphasises the importance of self-efficacy beliefs, causal attributions, and goal setting in regulating behaviour directed at accomplishing a task or activity. According to Zimmerman (2000), self-regulated learners have three important characteristics:

  • They use a variety of self-regulated strategies (active learning processes that involve agency and purpose).
  • They believe they can perform efficaciously.
  • They set numerous and varied goals for themselves.

Furthermore, self-regulated learners engage in three crucial processes:

  • Self-observation (monitoring of one's activities).
  • Self-judgment (evaluation of how well one's performance compares to a standard or the performance of others).
  • Selfreactions (reactions to performance outcomes).

.

Theories Linking Motivation and Cognition

Some motivation researchers are interested in how motivation and cognition interact to influence self-regulated learning.

Winne & Marx posited that motivation should be conceived in cognitive processing terms and that motivational thoughts and beliefs are governed by the basic principles of cognitive psychology, differing from other thoughts and beliefs only in their content. They further discuss the conditions under which tasks are performed, the operations needed to complete the task, the product produced when the task is completed, the evaluation of the task and how motivation can influence each aspect.

Borkowski and his colleagues developed a model highlighting the interaction of the following cognitive, motivational, and self-processes: knowledge of oneself, domain-specific knowledge, strategy knowledge, and personal-motivational states. This model incorporates several components, including entry characteristics; the social aspects of the learning setting; several motivational constructs derived from expectancy-value and goal theories; and various cognitive constructs.

Pintrich and colleagues postulated that the cognitive and motivational constructs influence each other as well as being influenced by the social context. In turn, both the cognitive and motivational constructs are assumed to influence involvement with learning and, consequently, achievement outcomes.

In support, Pintrich & De Groot found that achievement values determined initial engagement decisions. Their self-efficacy facilitated engagement and performance in conjunction with cognitive and self-regulation strategies. Pintrich et al. presented a more fully articulated discussion of links between motivation and cognition, explicitly referring to conceptual change. They discussed how traditional «cold» cognitive psychological models of conceptual change do not consider the motivational and contextual factors that likely influence conceptual development. They provided preliminary evidence of how motivational factors such as goals, achievement values, efficacy beliefs, and control beliefs can influence whether people change their mental concepts.

.

Theories of Motivation and Volition

The term "volition" refers to the strength of will needed to complete a task and the diligence of pursuit (Corno 1993). Kuhl (e.g., 1987) argued that many motivational theorists have ignored volitional processes by assuming that motivation leads directly to outcomes. He argued instead that motivational processes only lead to the decision to act. Once the individual engages in action, volitional processes take over and determine whether or not the intention is fulfilled. Finally, Kuhl proposed that some individuals are more likely to engage in these volitional strategies than «state-oriented» individuals.

Source
MOTIVATIONAL BELIEFS, VALUES, AND GOALS - J. Eccles - Annu. Rev. Psychol.- 2002